Investigação da relação entre a teoria da mente e as habilidades de pensamento criativo de crianças na área rural

Autores

Palavras-chave:

Pensamento divergente, Ambientes rurais, Crianças em idade escolar, Teoria da mente

Resumo

O objetivo deste estudo é determinar a relação entre criatividade e a teoria das habilidades mentais em crianças de 5 a 11 anos (Ns: 117) que continuam cursando o ensino fundamental em áreas rurais e revelar a relação entre as subdimensões dessas habilidades. No final do estudo, verificou-se que havia uma relação significativa e positiva entre o escore total de criatividade e as subdimensões da teoria da mente dessas crianças. Verificou-se também que havia uma relação significativa e positiva entre todas as subdimensões da criatividade e da teoria da mente. O escore total de criatividade e os escores das subdimensões da criatividade (fluência, originalidade e enriquecimento) foram preditores significativos, porém fracos, das tarefas da teoria da mente.

Downloads

Não há dados estatísticos.

Referências

Aslan, A. E., & Puccio, G. (2006). Developingand testing a turkish version of torrance tests of creative thinking: a study of adults. Journal of Creative Behavior, Massachusetts, 40(3), 163-178. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2162-6057.2006. tb01271.x

Astington J. W., & Edward M. J. (2010). The development of theory of mind in early childhood. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development. Canada: CEECD. Retrieved from http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/sites/default/files/ dossiers-complets/en/social-cognition.pdf#page=16

Ayçiçeği, A., Dinn, W. M., & Harris, C. L. (2003). Prefrontal lob nöropsikolojik test bataryası: Sağlıklı yetişkinlerden elde edilen test sonuçları. İ.Ü Studies in Psychology, 23, 1-26. Retrieved from http://dergipark.gov.tr/download/article-file/100078

Aydın, M. Ş., & Karakelle, S. (2016). The effects of theory of mind and self-regulation skills on helping behaviors in 3-4-year-old children. Nesne, 4(8), 205-226. https://doi.org/10.7816/nesne-04-08-03

Baldwin, D. (1991). Infants’ contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development, 62, 875-890. https:// doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01577.x

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, M.A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the Autistic child have a “Theory of Mind?” MRC Cognitive Development Unit, 21, 37-46. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8

Bensalah, L., Caillies, S., & Anduze, M. (2016). Links among cognitive empathy, theory of mind, and affective perspective taking by young children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 177(1), 17-31. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.20 15.1106438

Biçer, E., & Sarı, O. T. (2017). Examination of the relationship between theory of mind and executive function skills for students with normal development and students with mild mental disabilities. Journal of Theory and Practice in Education, 13(2), 261-276. https://dergipark.org.tr/tr/download/article-file/299171

Brüne, M., & Brüne-Cohrs, U. (2006) Theory of mind-evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and psychopathology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 437-455. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.08.001

Charness, G., List, J. A., Rustichini, A., Samek, A., & Van De Ven, J. (2019). Theory of mind among disadvantaged children: evidence from a field experiment. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 166, 174-194.

Craig, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Creativity and İmagination in autism and Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 319-326. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022163403479

Dağlıoğlu, H. E. (2011). The development and support of creativity in early childhood period. International Online Journal of Educational Sciences, 3(2), 594-618. Retrieved from http://www.acarindex.com/dosyalar/makale/ acarindex-1423904388.pdf

Dasi, M. G., Pons, F., & Bender, P. K. (2016). Imaginary companions, theory of mind and emotion understanding in young children. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 24(2), 186-197. https://doi.org/10.1080/13 50293X.2014.919778

Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2016). Measuring theory of mind across middle childhood: reliability and validity of the silent films and strange stories tasks. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 149, 23-40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. jecp.2015.07.011

Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2018). Family correlates of false belief understanding in early childhood: a meta-analysis. Child Development, 89(3), 971-987. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12682

Dietrich, A. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of creativity. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11(6), 1011-1026. https:// doi.org/10.3758/BF03196731

Ekerim, M., & Selçuk, B. (2018). Longitudinal predictors of vocabulary knowledge in Turkish children: the role of maternal warmth, inductive reasoning, and children’s inhibitory control. Early Education and Development, 29(3), 324-341. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2017.1407607

Ensor, R., Devine, R. T., Marks, A., & Hughes, C. (2014). Mothers’ cognitive references to 2-year-olds predict theory of mind at ages 6 and 10. Child Development, 85(3), 1222-1235. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12186

Fink, E., Begeer, S., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V., & Rosnay, M. (2015). Friends, friendlessness, and the social consequences of gaining a theory of mind. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 27-30. https://doi.org/10.1111/ bjdp.12080

Fisher, R. (2005). Teaching children to think. United Kingdom: Nelson Thornes.

Flavell, J. H. (2000). Development of childrens’ knowledge about the mental world. İnternational Journal of Behavioral Development, 24(1), 15-23. Retrieved from http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1080/016502500383421

Güven, Y., Ayvaz, E., & Göktaş, İ. (2019). Investigation of the relationship between theory of mind and social problem solving skills of preschool children. Erken Çocukluk Çalışmaları Dergisi, 3(1), 76-98. https://doi.org/10.24130/eccd-jecs.1967201931130

Hofmann, S. G., Doan, S. N., Sprung, M., Wilson, A., Ebesutani, C., Andrews, L. A., … Harris, P. L. (2016). Training children’s theory-of-mind: a meta-analysis of controlled studies. Cognition, 150, 200-212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. cognition.2016.01.006

Imuta, K., Henry, J. D., Slaughter, V., Selcuk, B., & Ruffman, T. (2016). Theory of mind and prosocial behavior in childhood: a meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 52(8), 1192. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000140

Karasar, N. (1995). Scientific Research Methodology. Ankara: Research Education Consultancy.

Keçeli Kaysılı, B., & Acarlar, F. (2011). The development of theory of mind according to false belief performance of children ages 3 to 5. Educational Sciences: Theory and Practice, 11(4), 1809-1826.

Keskin, B. (2005). The relationship betweentheory of mind, symbolic transformations ın pretend play, and children’s social competence. Florida: Florida State University.

Korucu, I., Selcuk, B., & Harma, M. (2017). Self-Regulation: Relations with theory of mind and social behaviour. Infant and Child Development, 26(3). https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1988

Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation in infancy: the origins of “theory of mind”. Psychological Review, 94, 412-426. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.94.4.412

Mar, R. A., Tackett, J. L., & Moore, C. (2010). Exposure to media and theory-of-mind development in preschoolers. Cognitive Development, 25(1), 69-78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.11.002

Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Das Gupta, M., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73(6), 1715-1726. https:// doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00501

Müller, U., Liebermann-Finestone, D. P., Carpendale, J. I., Hammond, S. I., & Bibok, M. B. (2012). Knowing minds, controlling actions: the developmental relations between theory of mind and executive function from 2 to 4 years of age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(2), 331-348.

Palut, B. (2006). Different thinking forms and learning. In A. Oktay & Ö. P. Unutkan (Eds.), Within an overview of the age of primary education (pp. 101-118). İstanbul: Morpa Kültür Yayınları.

Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that...” attribution of second-order false beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 437-471. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022- 0965(85)90051-7

Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4, 515-526. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076512

Rosnay, M., & Hughes, C. (2006). Conversation and theory of mind: do children talk their way to socio-cognitive understanding? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(1), 7-37. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151005X82901

Ruffman, T., Perner, J., & Parkin, L. (1999). How parenting style affects false belief understanding. Social Development, 8(3), 395-411. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00103

Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children’s and mothers’ mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, 73(3), 734-751. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00435

Russ, S. W., & Lee, A. W. (2019). Pretend play in the classroom. The importance of play in early childhood education: psychoanalytic, attachment, and developmental perspectives. New York: Routledge.

Selcuk, B., Yavuz, H. M., Etel, E., Harma, M., and Ruffman, T. (2018). Executive function and theory of mind as predictors of socially withdrawn behavior in institutionalized children. Social Development, 27(1), 109-124. https://onlinelibrary. wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/sode.12252

Sigirtmac, A. D. (2016). An investigation on the effectiveness of chess training on creativity and theory of mind development at early childhood. Educational Research and Reviews, 11(11), 1056-1063. https://doi.org/10.5897/ERR2016. 2676

Slaughter, V., Imuta, K., Peterson, C. C., & Henry, J. D. (2015). Meta-analysis of theory of mind and peer popularity in the preschool and early school years. Child Development, 86(4), 1159-1174. https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/ epdf/10.1111/cdev.12372

Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(5), 640-656. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892998562942

Suddendorf, T., & Fletcher-Flinn, C. M. (1999). Children’s divergent thinking improves when they understand false beliefs. Creativity Research Journal, 12(2), 115-128. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1202_4

Suddendorf, T., & Fletcher-Flin, C. M. (1997). Theory of mind and origin of divergent thinking. Journal of Creative Behavior, 31(3), 169-179. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2162-6057.1997.tb00789.x

Taymaz Sarı, O. (2013). Theory of Mind and Practical Applications (1st ed.) Eğiten Kitap: Ankara.

Wang, Z., Devine, R. T., Wong, K. K., & Hughes, C. (2016). Theory of mind and executive function during middle childhood across cultures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 149, 6-22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.028

Watson, A. C., Nixon, C. L., Wilson, A., & Capage, L. (1999). Social interaction skills and theory of mind in young children. Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 386. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.2.386

Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655-684. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00304

Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523-541. https://doi. org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x

Yağmurlu, B., Sanson, A., & Köymen, S. B. (2005). Effects of parenting and child temperament on the development of prosocial behavior: the mediating role of theory of mind. Turkish Journal of Psychology, 20(55), 1-20. Retrieved from http://www.turkpsikolojiyazilari.com/PDF/TPD/55/01-24.pdf

Yıldız, C., & Karman, G. N. (2017). Creativity and Perspective Taking in Early Childhood Period. Ankara University, Journal of Faculty of Educational Sciences, 50(2), 33-58. https://dspace.ankara.edu.tr/xmlui/bitstream/ handle/20.500.12575/47136/23118.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y

Youngblade, L. M., & Dunn, J. (1995). Individual differences in young children’s pretend play with mother and sibling: Links to relationships and understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs. Child Development, 66(5), 1472-1492. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00946.x

Ziv, M., Solomon, A., Strauss, S., & Frye, D. (2016). Relations between the development of teaching and theory of mind in early childhood. Journal of Cognition and Development, 17(2), 264-284. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.201 5.1048862

Downloads

Publicado

2023-02-07

Como Citar

CERRAH ÖZSEVGEÇ, L., & KONTAŞ AZAKLI, T. (2023). Investigação da relação entre a teoria da mente e as habilidades de pensamento criativo de crianças na área rural. Estudos De Psicologia, 38. Recuperado de https://puccampinas.emnuvens.com.br/estpsi/article/view/7296

Edição

Seção

PSICOLOGIA DO DESENVOLVIMENTO